The first link contained within the "Additional Resources"
section of the RAC Foundation document is:
The data contained within this site is vast, and the links
section within it is extremely comprehensive.
I took a look at what pops up under the heading of “Young drivers” as
this is potentially going to be my target audience for my intervention. As can be seen the data contained within (and
below is a mere drop in the ocean of what is available) is pretty compelling
reading. It would seem that the only
reason why the number of deaths involving young drivers has declined of late is
due to the fact that less of them, proportionately, are learning how to drive
(for a variety of reasons).
In 2015, nearly 15% of all car occupants killed or seriously
injured were young car drivers aged 17-25 years.
While driver age is a risk factor for collisions (with the
youngest new drivers at most risk), the experience drivers gain in the first
six months after passing their test plays a more significant role in reducing
their collision rates.
Evidence from a range of studies suggests that young drivers
may overrate their driving ability and see driving as a matter of ‘natural
talent’ which can be judged by how confident a driver feels. This has important
implications for understanding young drivers’ attitudes in relation to road safety.
There is little research evidence that increased formal
driver training, before, during and after learning to drive, improves safety. A
number of themes have emerged that offer the hope of improving the
effectiveness of training, in making training address the cognitive and
attitudinal aspects of driving.
International evidence on Graduated Driver Licensing (GDL)
indicates an overall effectiveness of GDL in reducing crashes of young drivers.
The proportion of young adults (aged 17-20) with a full driving
licence has decreased since the early 1990s. In 1995/97, 43 percent of those
aged 17-20 held a full licence, compared with a low of 27 percent in 2004 and
31 percent in 2011.
A Young Drivers Factsheet (RRCGB) in 2009 reported that
young drivers aged 17 to 24 years were over represented in car accident
statistics. In 2009, they accounted for
12 percent of all licence holders, but of all accidents, 26% (over 42,000)
involved at least one young car driver.
In 2009, young car drivers were most often driving straight
ahead immediately before the accident (46%)
In 2009, negotiating a curve, bending left or right,
accounted for twice the proportion of young car driver manoeuvres compared to
older car drivers prior to an accident. The figures were 14% for young car
drivers versus 7 percent for older car drivers. Most young drivers (43 percent)
were not at a junction when they were involved in an accident compared with 38
percent of older car drivers.
A survival analysis of the length of time to new drivers’
first accident found three factors were associated with longer ‘survival’
rates: increased age, driving experience (possibly driving in busy town centres
and in the rain) and a self-reported driving style characterised as ‘attentive,
careful, responsible and safe’.
There is a need for greater clarity about what needs to be
learned in order to drive safely and to encourage learners to take
responsibility for their learning, through effective progress reporting and
self-evaluation.
Learner drivers with a more tolerant attitude to their own
driving violations (many of them speed-related) tend to go on to have a higher
post-test accident liability, based on measures from the Attitudes to Driving
Violations Scale (ADVS).
The current arrangements for training and testing appear to
motivate drivers to apply for the test as soon as they think they have a
moderate chance of passing. In order to improve their safety on the roads,
learners and new drivers need to be encouraged to learn more than what is
currently tested – for example, getting experience of the full range of driving
conditions, such as night-time driving and driving in bad weather and on
motorways.
There is little research evidence that increased formal
driver training improves safety. A number of themes have emerged that offer the
hope of improving the effectiveness of training, one being the desirability of
improving the hazard perception skills of learner drivers.
National Travel Survey data indicates that the overall
proportion of young adults with a full driving licence has decreased since the
early 1990s. It also provides reasons for not learning to drive which vary
according to age with main reasons including not needing to drive, not being
interested in driving and costs of driving.
RRCGB data from 2009 (RRCGB Young Drivers Factsheet)
indicated that accidents involving young drivers were more likely to lead to a
greater number of casualties compared with older drivers. Most of the young
drivers involved in accidents were male. Men also made up a higher proportion
of casualties in accidents involving young drivers.
The timing of accidents in 2009 (RRCGB Young Drivers
Factsheet) involving young car drivers mirrored the patterns of all drivers
with most happening on Fridays and during morning and evening rush hours on
weekdays. Although more young driver accidents happened on Friday and Saturday
nights (8pm – 4am).
In 2009 (RRCGB Young Drivers Factsheet), most accidents
involving young drivers took place in urban locations but were less serious
than those in rural places. Young Drivers were also most likely to be driving
straight ahead and most often were not at a junction. These proportions were
only slightly higher than those for older drivers with the exception of
negotiating a curve.
In 2009 (RRCGB Young Drivers Factsheet), most accidents were
attributed to ‘failed to look properly’, when a contributory factor was
recorded. Young driver accidents were also more likely to be attributed to
factors related to inexperience, which reflects what is known about the
relative importance of inexperience in causing young driver accidents. There
were also differences in reported contributory factors for females and males.
While impairment by alcohol was a contributory factor in a very small
percentage of young driver accidents, young drivers were overrepresented
compared with older drivers.
Traffic crashes are the single greatest killer of 15-24 year
olds in OECD countries. It is estimated that over 8,500 young drivers of
passenger vehicles were killed in 2004. Death rates for young, novice drivers
have decreased in many countries in recent decades. However, these reductions
have mirrored overall improvements in road safety, and death rates for 18-24
year old drivers typically remain more than double those of older drivers
Data from the Netherlands, Sweden and the UK have shown that
young male drivers' relative risk of crash fatality, compared with that of
older drivers, has increased considerably over the last decade. This was
measured by fatal accidents per million kilometres driven. (Young Drivers: The
Road to Safety, 2006 OECD Transport Research Centre)
Two studies investigating the learning to drive process
(RSRR81 and RSRR 87) suggest that many young drivers were insufficiently
prepared for the driving test mainly because they lacked enough and
sufficiently varied driving experience (e.g. weather conditions, rural and
urban roads) . It seems that while most young drivers take some formal lessons,
many do not engage sufficiently with the learning to drive process particularly
in terms of using study materials and using their theoretical learning in their
driving practice. Overall, the evidence suggests that many young drivers do not
develop a good understanding of safe driving as part of the learning to drive
process.
A study of how a cohort of young adults learned to drive
(RSRR 81) in 2008 found that: Virtually all respondents (99%) took some
lessons with an Approved Driving Instructor (ADI). For all respondents, the
median value was 40 hours of lessons.
Evidence from a variety of sources suggests that some young
driver characteristics are more likely to be associated with unsafe driving
and/or accident liability. These include younger age and particular personality
characteristics (e.g. sensation seeking, external locus of control). Having an
accident may also lead to changes in driving style. In one study, young drivers
who had been involved in an accident reported that their driving was less
confident afterwards and for female drivers, also less decisive and more prone
to errors.
A range of studies identify problems related to the training
of young drivers, mainly related to the lack of motivation of young drivers to
take responsibility for improving their driving. This is related to the content
of driver training which does not address driving on a cognitive and
attitudinal level. Suggestions for how to address these problems related to
young driver attitudes and perceptions of driving are identified in some
studies. These include tapping into parental influence and working with young
drivers’ perceptions of safe driving, ‘good driving’ and driving cultures – for
example, the social and emotional aspects of driving identified in the RSRR 74
study.
The practical test focuses too much on a candidate’s ability
to control the car safely at the expense of other knowledge and skills. (RSRR
No.87)
Parents are an important long-term influence on young
drivers’ behaviour, and there is a need to encourage parents to reflect on what
messages they send to their children about driving and road safety. Information
and education should include efforts to identify and publicise the positive
behaviour of adolescents and young drivers, and to portray peer norms as
pro-safety. (RSWP No. 18)
Four critical gaps in current approaches to driver learning
and testing are evident:
1. The relevance gap. Few young people – pre-drivers,
learners or novices – see the standards in the test (and other rules of
driving) as relevant to ‘real driving’.
2. The measures gap. Young people have no good way of
measuring the competence of other drivers other than their own feeling of
safety, and have no good way of measuring their own competence as drivers other
than their feeling of confidence.
3. The incentives gap. There are a number of disincentives
for young people to spend longer improving their driving pre-test, and few real
incentives to carry on getting better after passing.
4. The motivations gap. A number of young people do not see
any real need to get better, as they start the learning process already
confident in their own talent which is reinforced by their rapidly mastering
physical control of the car. (RSRR No. 86)
Overall, the evidence suggests that pre-driver, driver
education and training provision and testing is not sufficiently effective in
helping young drivers to drive safely and reduce accident risk. The evidence to
support this come from a range of reviews of evidence (TRL INS005, TRL PPR529,
RSRR 97) and primary research studies discussed in the research findings
section (particularly RSRR 81 and RSRR 87). Because the accident rates of young
drivers are higher in the initial post-test period and declines sharply
thereafter, gaining driving experience post-test is the main cause of the
reduction of young driver accidents. Of course, this means that young drivers
are at continued risk in the immediate post-test period and training and
education measures do not seem to have a direct impact on reducing this risk.
Risky driving behaviours and factors related to these, such
as speed choice, which has been shown to be linked with accidents, are not
effectively addressed in the driving test. This contributes to the failure of
the driving test in helping to produce safe and competent drivers. (RSRR No.87)
It is interesting to read the above. Much of which confirms what swirls around the driving training industry in daily chatter. I think the point being made about driving experience is really important. My intensive driving courses which are 4-6 hours in duration, enable pupils to travel distances that would simply not be possible with traditional driving lessons. For example, residents in Boston can drive to Peterborough to experience fast moving dual-carriageways that do not exist in Boston, they get to experience large roundabouts that also do not exist. In the process of getting there, they can travel either on fast moving single-carriageway roads such as the A16, A17, A52 or they can choose to travel on windy rural roads. Likewise, Peterborough residents have the time to travel to Grantham and experience driving on hills that Peterborough does not have. I think these experiences for my pupils are incredibly valuable.
Regarding the points made in the research about the responsibility of a pupil for controlling how they learn it reminds me of a conversation recently with one of my younger pupils. On the de-brief of a training session where she had expressed satisfaction with how she performed, I asked her “And if I was to ask you Tash to give me 2 reasons why you felt you managed to do so well today, what would you say?” (I had no particular reason for asking for 2, I was just hoping that asking for 2 might require a bit more thought). Her reply after about 5 seconds thought was “I drove slower, and I concentrated more”. I thought to myself, if I could bottle that response up and sell it to my pupils, I would be a millionaire.
Regarding the points made in the research about the responsibility of a pupil for controlling how they learn it reminds me of a conversation recently with one of my younger pupils. On the de-brief of a training session where she had expressed satisfaction with how she performed, I asked her “And if I was to ask you Tash to give me 2 reasons why you felt you managed to do so well today, what would you say?” (I had no particular reason for asking for 2, I was just hoping that asking for 2 might require a bit more thought). Her reply after about 5 seconds thought was “I drove slower, and I concentrated more”. I thought to myself, if I could bottle that response up and sell it to my pupils, I would be a millionaire.
Other links on their website included:
Search by Local Authority where fatalities occurred between
1999-2010 http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-15975720
What rental car companies rent to under 21 drivers in the United States?
ReplyDeleteYou probably wouldn’t want to stop in the middle of the Death Valley, or somewhere in the highlands of the Rocky Mountains due to a car breakdown. Thus, a reliable and highly trusted supplier is what every potential driver is searching for. The list of vendors is very long, and it’s absolutely problematic to find really worthwhile rental car companies that allow under 21 drivers among them.
National, Thrifty, Avis, Hertz, Payless, Advantage, Alamo, Enterprise, Dollar, Budget and NU are those providers who are supportive of young drivers. Especially for you, we have listed all American under 21 car rental companies with their customer ratings See more here: car rental age 20 .